Charles
Fillmore, Discoverer of
Frame Semantics, Dies in SF at 84: He Figured Out How Framing Works 02/18/2014 Charles J. Fillmore, one of the world's greatest
linguists -- ever -- died last Thursday, February 13, at the age of 84 in San
Francisco. He was the discoverer of frame semantics, who did the essential
research on the nature of framing in thought and language. He discovered that
we think, largely unconsciously, in terms of conceptual frames -- mental
structures that organize our thought. Further, he found that every word is
mentally defined in terms of frame structures. Our current understanding of
"framing" in social and political discourse derives ultimately from
his research, whose importance stretches well beyond linguistics to social and
political thought -- and all of intellectual life. The world has lost a scholar
of the greatest significance.
[by WashingtonsBlog : Spy Agencies Manipulate
and Disrupt Web Discussions to Promote Propaganda and Discredit Government
Critics: The alternative media has documented
for 5 years that the government uses disinformation and
disruption
(and here)
on the web to discredit activists and manipulate public opinion, just like
it smears
traditional television and print reporters who question the
government too acutely. WeÕve long reported that the government censors and
manipulates social media. More proof here.
New Edward Snowden documents confirm that BritainÕs spy agency is doing
so.
As
Glenn Greenwald writes today: One of the many pressing stories that remains
to be told from the Snowden archive is how western intelligence
agencies are attempting to manipulate and control online discourse with extreme
tactics of deception and reputation-destruction. These agencies are attempting
to control, infiltrate, manipulate, and warp online discourse, and in doing
so, are compromising the integrity of the internet
itself. Among the core self-identified purposes of JTRIG are two tactics: (1)
to inject all sorts of false material onto the internet in order to
destroy the reputation of its targets; and (2) to use social
sciences and other techniques to manipulate online discourse and activism to
generate outcomes it considers desirable. To see how extremist these programs
are, just consider the tactics they boast of using to achieve those ends: Òfalse
flag operationsÓ (posting material to the internet and falsely attributing
it to someone else), fake victim blog posts (pretending to be
a victim of the individual whose reputation they want to destroy), and posting
Ònegative informationÓ on various forums. ] Read more ]
. . .
. . .
If
you are interested in how our understanding of framing in public discourse
developed, you need to know about Chuck.
Chuck's insights have had a profound effect on the fields of both linguistics
and cognitive science. As one of the earliest exponents of Noam Chomsky's
transformational grammar, Chuck discovered what is known as the
"transformational cycle" in 1963, even before Chomsky came up with
the idea of "deep structure." My own relationship with Chuck began in
that year, when he came to speak on that topic at the Indiana University
Graduate Linguistics Club. Ever gracious, he accepted my invitation as an
officer of the club and drove all the way from Columbus, Ohio to speak to our
graduate students for nothing more than an Indiana potluck dinner. I have
revered him ever since, and our lives and work have been intertwined over more
than 50 years.
By
1965, we both became convinced of the massive role of semantics in grammar, but
we came up with very different theories. My tack was to introduce formal logic
as semantics into linguistics in late 1963. But Chuck, with greater insight,
noticed that grammar is organized in terms of the most basic experiences of
everyday life, for example, action and perception.
Experiences
have a basic structure -- wholes with parts: Thus an action can have Agents,
their Acts, Patients (what they act on), Purposes, Instruments, Locations,
Times, and so on.
Perception involves an Experiencer, and experience, a
Stimulus of the Experience, and so on. He called these conceptual elements of
experience "cases" on an analogy with case languages like Latin and
Greek. He called his theory "Case Grammar," showing that there are rules
of grammar that crucially make use of such very general conceptual elements
that structure our experience. I heard him speak on the idea at MIT in the
summer of 1965, and began following his development of the theory. He published
the idea in 1968, and the idea spread. A version of that idea is now taken for
granted pretty much throughout the linguistic world, partly though Chuck's work
and partly through a 1965 MIT dissertation by Jeff Gruber, who left linguistics
shortly thereafter to become a Baha'i missionary. In the cognitive tradition
following Chuck, they are called "semantic roles." In the generative
tradition, they are called "theta-roles." The insights are similar
and were discovered independently at about the same time.
Chuck
arrived at Berkeley in 1971, and I followed in 1972. We began working together,
as well as taking part in a cognition discussion group that included Dan Slobin, Eleanor Rosch, Wally
Chafe, Paul Kay, Steve Palmer, John Gumperz, and
occasionally, Paul Grice. That became the core of cognitive science at Berkeley.
When the field was formed later in the 1970's, Berkeley became the West Coast
center of the field. In 1974-75, while Chuck was developing frame semantics and
I was helping, we were regularly visited in my living room by
three friends who drove over from Palo Alto -- Terry Winograd,
Danny Bobrow, and Don Norman. They wanted to
find out what they could about the details of frame semantics since they were
working on a knowledge representation language for computer science, which
eventually developed into KL-ONE -- a classic frame-based knowledge
representation language in computer science. It was because of Chuck that
it came to be "frame-based."
In
1975, Chuck published his first paper on frame semantics in the first issue of
the Publications of the Berkeley Linguistics Society, and in 1976 published a
second version in 1976 in The Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. Frame
semantics was a much-elaborated version of case grammar. Chuck had been
studying European linguistic research on "semantic fields" -- groups
of related words like knife-fork-spoon, Sunday, Monday, Tuesday, ... and so on.
Chuck realized that they were also based on organized mental structures of
common experiences, but he went a major step further: the hypothesis that every
word in every language is mentally defined by elements of such mental
structures, which he called "frames." Chuck's classic example
involved the semantic field buy-sell-goods-price-cost. The common mental
structure defining such words is based on the commercial event scenario: Person
1 has possessions and wants to exchange them for money. Person 2 has the money
and wants to exchange it for such a possession. There is mutual exchange.
Person 1 is called a seller; Person 2 is called a buyer; the possession
exchanged is called the goods; and the money is called the price. Those named
the basic "semantic roles" -- the conceptual elements of the frame. [see ÒscriptingÓ]
Being
Chuck, he went further. Sentences that looked very different have meanings
characterized by the same frame. Chuck sold the book to Paul for $10. Paul
bought the book from Chuck for $10. The book cost Paul $10. Chuck got $10 for
the book. Moreover, each verb defined by that frame has its own grammar
associated with it. With sell, the Seller is Subject, the goods is direct object, the buyer is marked by to and the price is
marker by for. With cost, the goods is subject, the
price is direct object and the Buyer is indirect object. This is a very
simple example. There is a great deal more to frames. In the 18 years Chuck worked on the FrameNet
project, over a thousand frames were described in detail. They are publicly
accessible on the web at www.framenet.icsi.berkeley.edu.
The
study of frame semantics became the study of (1) which frames do we use in
conceptualizing our experience, (2) what semantic roles and scenarios define
each frame, (3) what words are defined by which frames, (4) what is the grammar
associated with the frame elements, and (5) how are frames related to one
another. I was hooked on frame semantics by 1975, and started working
with Chuck on a linguistic theory that would incorporate frames. We called it
Construction Grammar, providing a unified theory
of grammar and word meaning. In 1978, Michel Reddy and I, independently, found
evidence that metaphor was not just in language, but
in thought. We think to a remarkable extent in metaphor, and that
metaphorical concepts, like frames, are largely unconscious. [ See men vs.women
] Having worked with Chuck, I realized that conceptual
metaphors were frame-to-frame mappings, ways of understanding one area of
framed experience in terms of another. A year later, Mark Johnson and I came to
the conclusion that frames, metaphors, and all other aspects of thought are
based on what we called "embodiment," postulating a theory of
embodied cognition. Having followed Chuck's instincts on the role of everyday
embodied experience in both case grammar and frame semantics, this seemed
natural to me. Embodied
cognition has become a major research area in the cognitive
sciences.
Chuck,
working with his close friend Paul Kay, came up with a version of Construction
Grammar fitting FrameNet goals and methods. I came up
with an embodied version of Construction Grammar that took into account
conceptual metaphor, embodied aspects of frames and metaphors, and the idea of
conceptual prototypes. We published elaborate initial papers on our versions of
construction grammar at virtually the same time. Chuck also inspired the research I have done over many years
in applying frame semantics to politics. In 1977, Chuck told me about a court
case in Boston in which a doctor who had performed an abortion was put on trial
for murder. In the trial, the defense attorney used the word fetus and the
prosecuting attorney used the word baby. Fetus invoked the frame of a
medical procedure, while baby invoked a killing frame. The medical frame
won out in the trial. But the point was not lost on me: competing frames are
used everywhere in political and social issues and who wins depends on which
frame dominates. To understand exactly how conceptual framing works through
language, the appropriate field of study is frame semantics.
Charles J. Fillmore was the man who first figured out how framing works.
He is world-renowned in linguistics, but deserves a much wider appreciation as
a major intellectual. I have cited his work over and over, in my writing and in
my talks. But over more than 50 years, he worked
modestly as an OWL, an ordinary working linguist. He was brought up in St.
Paul, Minnesota, and was known for his Minnesotan modesty, gentlemanliness, and
a sly wit befitting Lake Woebegone. When he first came to Berkeley in 1971, he
encountered a culture defined by the then-commonplace
expression, "Let it all hang out." His response was to wear a button
saying, "Tuck it all back in." I will always miss him. George Lakoff
is Distinguished Professor of Cognitive Science and
Linguistics at the University of California, Berkeley. His website is: www.georgelakoff.com
Check out
a related socio-linguist: Robin Lakoff.